Central governor

From Self-sufficiency
Revision as of 20:21, 14 September 2010 by 85.230.205.253 (Talk)

(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Jump to: navigation, search
File:Jon Tvedt.JPG
The Norwegian mountain runner Jon Tvedt engaging in a strenuous run: it is suggested that the central governor ensures that such endurance exertion does not threaten the body's homeostasis

The central governor is a proposed process in the brain that regulates exercise in regard to a neurally calculated safe exertion by the body. In particular, physical activity is controlled so that its intensity cannot threaten the body’s homeostasis by causing anoxia damage to the heart. The central governor limits exercise by reducing the neural recruitment of muscle fibres. This reduced recruitment is experienced as fatigue. The existence of a central governor was suggested to explain fatigue after prolonged strenuous exercise in marathons and other endurance sports, but its ideas could also apply to other causes of exertion fatigue.

The existence of a central governor was proposed by Tim Noakes in 1997, but a similar idea was suggested in 1924 by Archibald Hill.

In contrast to this idea is the one that fatigue is due to peripheral ‘limitation’ or ‘catastrophe’. In this view, regulation by fatigue occurs as a consequence of a failure of homoeostasis directly in muscles.

Hill

The 1922 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine winner Archibald Hill proposed in 1924 that the heart was protected from anoxia in strenuous exercise by the existence of a governor.

the heart is able to regulate its output, to some extent, in accordance with the degree of saturation of the arterial blood … we suggest that, in the body (either in the heart muscle itself or in the nervous system), there is some mechanism which causes a slowing of the circulation as soon as a serious degree of unsaturation occurs, and vice versa. This mechanism would tend, to some degree, to act as a ‘governor’, maintaining a reasonably high degree of saturation of the blood: the breathing of a gas mixture rich in oxygen would produce a greater degree of saturation of the blood and so allow the output to increase until the ‘governor’ stopped it again. We realise the danger of a hypothesis partly suggested by teleological reasoning: in this case, however, we can see no other explanation of our experimental results pp. 161-163[1]

This hypothesis was disregarded and further research upon exercise fatigue was modelled in terms of it being due to a mechanical failure of the exercising muscles ("peripheral fatigue"). This failure was caused either by an inadequate oxygen supply to the exercising muscles, lactic acid build up, or total energy depletion in the exhausted muscles.[2]

Noakes

Tim Noakes, a professor of exercise and sports science at the University of Cape Town, in 1997[3] has renewed Hill’s argument on the basis of modern research.

In his approach, the power output by muscles during exercise is continuously adjusted in regard to calculations made by the brain in regard to a safe level of exertion. These neural calculations factor in earlier experience with strenuous exercise, the planning duration of the exercise, and the present metabolic state of the body. These brain models[4] ensure that body homeostasis is protected, and an emergency reserve margin is maintained.[5][6][7][8] This neural control adjusts the number of activated skeletal muscle motor units, a control which is subjectively experienced as fatigue. This process, though occurring in the brain, is outside personal control.

the rising perception of discomfort produced by exhausting exercise progressively reduces the conscious desire to over-ride this control mechanism, which, if it were to be reduced, would lead to the recruitment of more motor units. Thus the presence of conscious over-ride would be undesirable because it would increase or maintain the exercise intensity, thereby threatening homoeostasis… as exercise performance is centrally regulated by the CNS, then fatigue should no longer be considered a physical event but rather a sensation or emotion, separate from an overt physical manifestation—for example, the reduction in force output by the active muscles. Rather we now suggest that the physical manifestation of any increasing perception of fatigue may simply be an alteration in the subconsciously regulated pace at which the exercise is performed. Hence the novel suggestion is that the conventional understanding of fatigue is flawed because it makes no distinction between the sensation itself and the physical expression of that sensation which, we suggest, is the alteration in the subconsciously regulated pacing strategy consequent on changing motor unit recruitment/derecruitment by the CNS.[9]

Other areas

Noakes created the idea of the central governor in the context of prolonged endurance running. However, he has noted that the central processes involved might also underlie the existence of other kinds of fatigue:

This new interpretation is the first to allow a more reasonable description of a number of phenomena that defy rational explanation according to the traditional ‘‘limitations’’ models of fatigue. These include, among many others, the chronic fatigue syndrome, in which affected individuals experience evident fatigue at rest, and the role of psychological and motivational factors, centrally (brain) acting pharmaceutical agents, hypnosis, shouting or sudden unexpected gunshots, or other forms of distraction including music or premeditated deception on human exercise performance.[10]

In support of this, placebos (which must be meditated by a central process) have a powerful effect upon not only fatigue in prolonged exercise,[11][12] but also upon short term endurance exercise such as sprint speed,[13] the maximum weight that could be lifted with leg extension,[14] and the tolerance of ischemic pain and power when a tourniqueted hand squeezes a spring exerciser 12 times.[15]

Criticisms

The existence of a central governor over physiology has been questioned since ‘physiological catastrophes’ can and do occur in athletes (important examples in marathons have been Dorando Pietri, Jim Peters and Gabriela Andersen-Schiess). This suggests that humans can over-ride ‘the central governor’.[16] Moreover, a variety of peripheral factors in addition to such those such as lactic acid build up can impair muscle power and might act to protect against "catastrophe".[17] Another objection is that models incorporating conscious control also provide an alternative explanation,[18] but also see Noakes’ reply.[19]

Exercise fatigue has also been attributed to the direct effects of exercise upon the brain such as increased cerebral levels of serotonin, reduced level of glutamate secondary to uptake of ammonia in the brain, brain hyperthermia, and glycogen depletion in brain cells.[20][21]

The idea of exercise causing hypoxia at the heart, in the absence of arterial disease, moreover can be questioned due to the heart with every beat is delivering through coronary arteries that arise from first branches from the aorta freshly oxygenated blood to its own cells. Other factors exist that could in a self limiting way limit oxygen uptake. For example, as more accessory muscles of respiration are recruited, (as occurs at near maximal values of VO2), the energy cost of increasing rib cage expansion is nearly equal to that gained by the oxygen obtained from doing so. Indeed, the Fick equation (see VO2 max) itself includes terms of limitation: Q (cardiac output) is determined by stroke volume and heart rate. Stroke volume has a natural, physically limited upper bound (the heart obviously has a maximal volume, and is restricted by surrounding structures such as the pericardium), while heart rate is limited by the ability rate at which cardiac cells can maintain rhythmicity. There are also natural limits to the rate at which oxygen can diffuse from the blood to the tissues, i.e. gas exchange is itself a limiting factor.

These criticisms suggest the potential exists for known physiological processes to adequately carry out what Noakes and others attribute to a complex, pre-calculated central mechanism of homeostasis. (Though they may be relevant for accounting for some types of observations such as the effects of altitude on cardiac and other muscular capabilities.)

Another criticism is that Hill's original suggestion of a central governor uses a study in which a VO2 max test was conducted in which some of the subjects did not achieve a plateau in oxygen uptake. This failure led to his suggestion that VO2 max itself is a failure to account for their fatigue requiring the existence of another mechanism that could limit aerobic performance. However, this plateau requires that subjects are highly motivated, as the protocol of the test requires work at near maximal levels for protracted periods, and this might not have been the case.

See also

Notes

Cite error: Invalid <references> tag; parameter "group" is allowed only.

Use <references />, or <references group="..." />
  1. Hill, A. V., Long, C. N. H. and Lupton, H. (1924). Muscular exercise, lactic acid and the supply and utilisation of oxygen. Parts I–III. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 97, 438–475.
  2. Edwards RHT. (1983) Biochemical bases for fatigue in exercise performance: catastrophe theory in muscular fatigue. In: Biochemistry of exercise. Knuttgen HG, Vogel JA, Poortmans J, eds. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1–28. ISBN 978-0608071121
  3. Noakes, T. D. (1997) 1996 J. B. Wolffe Memorial Lecture. Challenging beliefs: ex Africa semper aliquid novi. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 29, 571–590. PubMed
  4. St Clair Gibson, A., Baden, D. A., Lambert, M. I., Lambert, E. V., Harley, Y. X., Hampson, D., Russell, V. A. Noakes, T. D. (2003) "The conscious perception of the sensation of fatigue". Sports Med. 33: 167-176 PubMed
  5. Noakes, T. D., St Clair Gibson, A. Lambert, E. V. (2005) "From catastrophe to complexity: a novel model of integrative central neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in humans: summary and conclusions". Br J Sports Med. 39: 120-124 PubMed
  6. Noakes, T. D., Peltonen, J. E. Rusko, H. K. (2001) "Evidence that a central governor regulates exercise performance during acute hypoxia and hyperoxia". J Exp Biol. 204: 3225-3234 PubMed
  7. Noakes, T. D. (2000) "Physiological models to understand exercise fatigue and the adaptations that predict or enhance athletic performance". Scand J Med Sci Sports. 10: 123-145 PubMed
  8. St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. L. Noakes, T. D. (2001) "Neural control of force output during maximal and submaximal exercise". Sports Med. 31: 637-650 PubMed
  9. St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. L. Noakes, T. D. (2001) "Neural control of force output during maximal and submaximal exercise". Sports Med. 31: 637-650 PubMed pp 121-122.
  10. St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, M. L. Noakes, T. D. (2001) "Neural control of force output during maximal and submaximal exercise". Sports Med. 31: 637-650 PubMed page 121
  11. Clark, V.R., Hopkins, W.G., Hawley, J.A. & Burke, L.M. (2000) Placebo effect of carbohydrate feeding during a 40-km cycling time trial. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 32, 1642–1647
  12. Foad, A.J., Beedie, C.J. & Coleman, D.A. (2008) Pharmacological and psychological effects of caffeine ingestion in 40-km cycling performance. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc., 40, 158–165
  13. Beedie, C.J., Coleman, D.A. & Foad, A.J. (2007) Positive and negative placebo effects resulting from the deceptive administration of an ergogenic aid. Int. J. Sport Nutr. Exerc. Metab., 17, 259–269
  14. Pollo A, Carlino E, Benedetti F. (2008) The top-down influence of ergogenic placebos on muscle work and fatigue. Eur J Neurosci. 28, 379-88.
  15. Benedetti, F., Pollo, A. & Colloca, L. (2007) Opioid-mediated placebo responses boost pain endurance and physical performance – is it doping in sport competitions? J. Neurosci., 27, 11934–11939.
  16. Esteve-Lanao, J., Lucia, A., deKoning, J. J. Foster, C. (2008) "How do humans control physiological strain during strenuous endurance exercise?" PLoS ONE. 3: e2943 PubMed
  17. Weir, J. P., Beck, T. W., Cramer, J. T. Housh, T. J. (2006) "Is fatigue all in your head? A critical review of the central governor model". Br J Sports Med. 40: 573-586; discussion 586 PubMed
  18. Marcora, S. M. (2008) "Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance?" Eur J Appl Physiol. 104: 929-931PubMed
  19. Noakes, T. D. Tucker, R. (2008) "Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance? A response to the letter of Dr. Marcora". Eur J Appl Physiol. 104: 933-935 PubMed
  20. Dalsgaard, M. K. (2006) "Fuelling cerebral activity in exercising man". J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 26: 731-750 PubMed
  21. Dalsgaard, M. K. Secher, N. H. (2007) "The brain at work: a cerebral metabolic manifestation of central fatigue?" J Neurosci Res. 85: 3334-3339 PubMed